Showing posts with label Neuroscience. Show all posts
Showing posts with label Neuroscience. Show all posts

21 June 2023

Brain Injury May Be A Factor In Soldier Suicides

Once again, because it is invisible, brain injury is underestimated as a serious source of harm.
A far-reaching study by Navy researchers has found that exposure to munitions blast waves from combat and training may be causing brain injuries the aggregation of which is resulting serious and often deadly ailments such as depression, PTSD and suicide.
From here.

23 May 2023

Brain Injury Is Behind Many Of Society's Worst Problems

[N]early two-thirds of those experiencing chronic homelessness in metro Denver suffer from some kind of brain injury[.]
From here.

The very bottom of American society, from the people who are "problem" inmates in jails and prisons, to the chronically homeless, is pervasively full of people with brain injuries. And, mostly, these are conditions that can't be cured.

24 April 2023

Addiction Is Genetic Predisposition Plus

Most people who try drugs don’t get addicted, even to opioids or methamphetamine, which suggests that ‌factors other than simply being exposed to a drug can contribute to addiction. ‌The majority of people who do get hooked have other psychiatric disorders, traumatic childhoods or both — only ‌7 percent report no history of mental illness. ‌‌Nearly 75 percent of women with heroin addiction‌‌ were sexually abused as children — and most people with any type of addiction have suffered at least one and often many forms of childhood trauma‌‌.
Chandra Sripada, professor of psychiatry and philosophy at the University of Michigan, argues that distorted thinking is more important in addictive behavior than overwhelming desire, leading to what he calls “unreliable” control over use. . . .  During addiction . . . despairing thoughts about oneself and the future — not just thoughts about how good the drug is — predominate. At the same time, thoughts about negative consequences of use are minimized, as are those about alternative ways of coping. Drugs are overvalued as a way to mitigate distress; everything else is undervalued. The result is an unstable balance, which, more often than not, tips toward getting high.

This theory ‌is helpful for explaining who is most likely to get addicted and what is most likely to generate recovery. Risk factors like poverty, a traumatic childhood and mental illness generate excess stress while tending to produce negative thoughts about oneself. In my case, I was depressed and isolated because of what I later learned was undiagnosed autism spectrum disorder — and hated myself for my inability to connect. The result was a mental climate conducive to relying on drugs, even when they no longer ‌provided relief.

Factors linked to recovery — like social support and employment — can offset distorted thoughts and inflated valuation of drug use. Essentially, people make better choices when they recognize and have access to better options‌. If you are locked in a room with an escape route unknown to you hidden under the carpet, you are just as trapped as if that exit didn’t exist. My recovery began when I saw that there was a bearable way out.

This is why punitive approaches so often backfire: Causing more pain to people who view drugs as their only way to cope drives desire to use even more. Punishment doesn’t teach new skills that can allow better decisions.

From the New York Times

The story has many other worthwhile insights, for example, on how addition is neither a total absence of free will nor a totally unfettered free choice.

28 September 2022

The Genetics Of Autism and ADHD

A new study compared the genetic profiles of people diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and people diagnosed with both disorders. Both conditions are highly heritable. 

The study used large data set with 23,583 subjects (with ADHD and/or one of four subtypes of autism spectrum disorder) and 42,201 controls, apparently mostly from the national health records of Denmark and possibly also from 23andMe data. The study excluded individuals with a moderate to severe mental retardation from both its subjects and its controls.

The study finds seven genes that are associated with both disorders, and five genes that distinguish between the disorders which are also associated with educational attainment, neuroticism and regional brain volume. 

The Paper And Its Abstract
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are highly heritable neurodevelopmental conditions, with considerable overlap in their genetic etiology. We dissected their shared and distinct genetic etiology by cross-disorder analyses of large datasets. 
We identified seven loci shared by the disorders and five loci differentiating them. All five differentiating loci showed opposite allelic directions in the two disorders and significant associations with other traits, including educational attainment, neuroticism and regional brain volume. 
Integration with brain transcriptome data enabled us to identify and prioritize several significantly associated genes. The shared genomic fraction contributing to both disorders was strongly correlated with other psychiatric phenotypes, whereas the differentiating portion was correlated most strongly with cognitive traits. Additional analyses revealed that individuals diagnosed with both ASD and ADHD were double-loaded with genetic predispositions for both disorders and showed distinctive patterns of genetic association with other traits compared with the ASD-only and ADHD-only subgroups. 
These results provide insights into the biological foundation of the development of one or both conditions and of the factors driving psychopathology discriminatively toward either ADHD or ASD.
Manuel Mattheisen, et al., "Identification of shared and differentiating genetic architecture for autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and case subgroups" Nat Genet (September 26, 2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01171-3 (closed access).

The Data Set

The data set was apparently a national health care record system for Denmark in which there were 2,304 cases with both ADHD ad an autism spectrum disorder, 11,964 ADHD cases without an autism spectrum disorder, and 9,315 cases with an autism spectrum disorder without ADHD, although some of the data may also have been from the 23andme consumer genetic profiling company (a data set that would include me, my family, and a number of my extended family members). Also:
Controls were randomly selected from the full control cohort to roughly match a 1:4 ratio in cases and controls. . . . we excluded individuals with a moderate to severe mental retardation (ICD10: F71-F79) from both the case and control cohort.

Autism spectrum disorder cases were further categoried into four subtypes: childhood autism (cha, ICD10 F84.0); atypical autism (ata, ICD10 F84.1); Asperger’s syndrome (asp, ICD10 F84.5); and pervasive disorders, unspecified and others (pdm, ICD10 F84.8+9). 

It appears that ADHD cases were not subtyped, which is something of a shame. There is good reasons to think that the genetic basis of ADHD predominantly inattentive type (i.e. without hyperactivity), has a different genetic basis than ADHD combined type (and ADHD predominately hyperactive which probably overlaps heavily with the combined type). This level of information ought to have been available although empirically efforts to further subtype ADHD probably wouldn't have been available in the data. See, e.g, prior posts at this blog from September 8, 2012, April 3, 2012, and October 18, 2017 (Genetics of ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention dimensions are quite different),

A Key Figure From The Paper


A figure from the closed access paper:


Fig. 2: Comparison of PRS profiles across ADHD and ASD subtypes for 15 traits and/or phenotypes that have shown significant genetic correlations with ADHD and ASD in the past.

Peer Review

The relatively new practice of disclosing comments from the peer review of the paper, provides some interesting insights although it is rather technical and difficult to evaluate without an open access manuscript to reference it to for the most part. Some notable comment state:
A serious limitation of this study is that the design relies on the common psychiatric nosology that in particular for ASD and ADHD has been problematic, surely in the past when only one of both diagnosis was allowed. Given the high comorbidity between ADHD and ASD, and among other psychiatric disorders in general, plus the fact that recent research shows that ‘genes do not respect diagnostic classifications’ (see CDG publications) I would rather see a focus on the shared genetic findings for ADHD and ASD, in aiming to find general genetic/biological vulnerabilities for trans diagnostic neurodevelopmental problems, instead of relying on a classification system that has surely proven its value in clinical practice but does not seem to guide biological underpinnings of those disorders.

and

Concerning the used sample, I would appreciate getting some more details on the assessment of comorbid ASD and ADHD in the respective cohort. While some of the individuals might have been diagnosed under DSM-IV, which did not allow a comorbid diagnosis of ADHD, I wonder if and how this was accounted for in the datasets. In light of the cited Meta-study 25-32% of ASD individuals do fulfill the criteria for ADHD. In this study, the comorbid cohort accounts for 10% of the sample (assuming they were from both the ASD and the ADHD cohorts).

The responses gave rise to a table regarding the shared risk genes:

26 November 2021

Human 2.0: Executive Function Boosting

Another high risk, high reward concept from DARPA:
Researchers show it is possible to improve specific human brain functions related to self-control and mental flexibility by merging artificial intelligence with targeted electrical brain stimulation. . . . 
[T]hey identified a brain region -- the internal capsule -- that improved patients' mental function when stimulated with small amounts of electrical energy. That part of the brain is responsible for cognitive control -- the process of shifting from one thought pattern or behavior to another, which is impaired in most mental illnesses.
An example might include a person with depression who just can't get out of a "stuck" negative thought. Because it is so central to mental illness, finding a way to improve it could be a powerful new way to treat those illnesses.
The team developed algorithms, so that after stimulation, they could track patients' cognitive control abilities, both from their actions and directly from their brain activity. The controller method provided boosts of stimulation whenever the patients were doing worse on a laboratory test of cognitive control.
This system can read brain activity, "decode" from that when a patient is having difficulty, and apply a small burst of electrical stimulation to the brain to boost them past that difficulty. The analogy I often use is an electric bike. When someone's pedaling but having difficulty, the bike senses it and augments it. We've made the equivalent of that for human mental function.
The study is the first to show that:
* A specific human mental function linked to mental illness can be reliably enhanced using precisely targeted electrical stimulation; 
* There are specific sub-parts of the internal capsule brain structure that are particularly effective for cognitive enhancement; and 
* A closed-loop algorithm used as a controller was twice as effective than stimulating at random times.

Some of the patients had significant anxiety in addition to their epilepsy. When given the cognitive-enhancing stimulation, they reported that their anxiety got better, because they were more able to shift their thoughts away from their distress and focus on what they wanted. Widge says that this suggests this method could be used to treat patients with severe and medication-resistant anxiety, depression or other disorders. 
. . .  
The research team is now preparing for clinical trials. Because the target for improving cognitive control is already approved by the Food and Drug Administration for deep brain stimulation, . . .  this research can be done with existing tools and devices -- once a trial is formally approved -- and the translation of this care to current medical practice could be rapid.
From Science Daily citing Ishita Basu, et al., "Closed-loop enhancement and neural decoding of cognitive control in humans." Nature Biomedical Engineering (2021) DOI: 10.1038/s41551-021-00804-y

07 July 2020

Brain Scans Lend Insight Into Six Common Psychiatric Conditions

We are starting to figure out how psychiatric disorders previously classified solely by symptoms are related to each other (and different from each other) at a biological level in the brain. 
"We found that 4 major psychiatric disorders -- major depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and obsessive-compulsive disorder -- show a surprisingly high level of similarity in their brain structural abnormalities," said Dr. Opel. The shared brain areas showing structural aberrations were mainly in cortical areas associated with cognitive processing, memory and self-awareness. 
On the flipside, Dr. Opel added, "we were able to identify regional abnormalities with high specificity for certain disorders." Interestingly, these distinct structural differences sometimes appeared in the same area for two disorders, but in opposite directions from the norm. 
In contrast, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder did not share brain structural signatures with any other disorders. That may be because those disorders are considered developmental diseases with a distinct etiology from the other psychiatric conditions, which have more in common. 
The researchers do not yet understand the mechanisms behind the shared structural elements, but a growing body of evidence shows that these psychiatric disorders also share common genetic as well as environmental influences, which might underlie the current findings.
From Science Daily. The paper and its abstract are as follows:
Background 
Neuroimaging studies have consistently reported similar brain structural abnormalities across different psychiatric disorders. Yet, the extent and regional distribution of shared morphometric abnormalities between disorders remains unknown. 
Methods 
Here, we conducted a cross-disorder analysis of brain structural abnormalities in 6 psychiatric disorders based on effect size estimates for cortical thickness and subcortical volume differences between healthy control subjects and psychiatric patients from 11 mega- and meta-analyses from the ENIGMA (Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics Through Meta Analysis) consortium. Correlational and exploratory factor analyses were used to quantify the relative overlap in brain structural effect sizes between disorders and to identify brain regions with disorder-specific abnormalities. 
Results 
Brain structural abnormalities in major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and obsessive-compulsive disorder were highly correlated ( r = .443 to r = .782), and one shared latent underlying factor explained between 42.3% and 88.7% of the brain structural variance of each disorder. The observed shared morphometric signature of these disorders showed little similarity with brain structural patterns related to physiological aging. In contrast, patterns of brain structural abnormalities independent of all other disorders were observed in both attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Brain regions showing high proportions of independent variance were identified for each disorder to locate disorder-specific morphometric abnormalities. 
Conclusions 
Taken together, these results offer novel insights into transdiagnostic as well as disorder-specific brain structural abnormalities across 6 major psychiatric disorders. Limitations comprise the uncertain contribution of risk factors, comorbidities, and medication effects to the observed pattern of results that should be clarified by future research.

03 February 2020

Study: Autism Basically The Inverse of M.S.

NPR has a story that indicates that autism may be basically a function of too much mylenination (the body's natural nerve insulation), while M.S. is too little, according to a new study.

On the bright side, we are much further along in finding medical treatments for M.S., than for autism for which treatment has been basically intractable, and the new understanding of autism may shed light on how to treat it derived from M.S. research.

18 December 2018

Links Between Mental Health And Physical Health

Eczema patients at 36% higher risk of suicide attempts.

* Infections in kids tied to subsequent mental illness risk.
The study, published in the journal JAMA Psychiatry on Wednesday, found that infections requiring hospitalizations were associated with an about 84% increased risk of being diagnosed with any mental disorder and an about 42% increased risk of using psychotropic drugs to treat a mental disorder. Less severe infections treated with anti-infective medications, like antibiotics, were associated with increased risks of 40% and 22%, respectively, the study found. . . .
The researchers found associations between any treated infection and the increased risk of later being prescribed medication for various childhood and adolescent mental disorders, with the risks differing for specific disorders. 
Risks were increased for schizophrenia spectrum disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorder, personality and behavior disorders, mental retardation, autism spectrum disorders, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder and tic disorders[.]
The last sentence of the first paragraph of the quotation from the news story above doesn't really make sense. The reason for the correlation is not known. The study does not support the claim that not treating an infection will prevent mental illness.

The data involve 1 million people in Denmark born from 1995 to 2012, which centralized health records make this kind of big data research possible and reliable. It is basically measuring childhood mental health conditions rather than adult conditions.

03 July 2017

Chemical Target For Extending Child-Like Language Acquisition Identified

By disrupting adenosine signaling in the auditory thalamus, we have extended the window for auditory learning for the longest period yet reported, well into adulthood and far beyond the usual critical period in mice," said corresponding author Stanislav Zakharenko, M.D., Ph.D., a member of the St. Jude Department of Developmental Neurobiology. "These results offer a promising strategy to extend the same window in humans to acquire language or musical ability by restoring plasticity in critical regions of the brain, possibly by developing drugs that selectively block adenosine activity."
The paper is:

Jay A. Blundon, et al., "Restoring auditory cortex plasticity in adult mice by restricting thalamic adenosine signaling." Science, 2017 DOI: 10.1126/science.aaf4612

28 June 2017

Second Trimester Fevers Increase Autism Risks 40%

A new study finds that fevers in pregnant women during the second trimester increase the risk of a child with autism by 40%. The magnitude of this effect closely matches estimates from previous independent studies. The sample size is 95,000 (which is just barely big enough to measure these effects), while most other fevers are not as definitvely associated with autism risk.
The link is strongest in the second trimester, when a single fever is associated with a 40 percent increase in autism risk. Three or more fevers after the first trimester triples the risk of having a child with autism, according to the study, which appeared 13 June in Molecular Psychiatry. 
The findings support the idea that a pregnant woman’s immune response, which often includes fever, can disrupt brain development in the fetus, says lead researcher Mady Hornig, associate professor epidemiology at the Columbia University. 
The study is inconclusive on whether drugs that lower fever mitigate the risk, but the results hint that they might, says Sarkis Mazmanian, professor of biology at the 
California Institute of Technology. . . .

15,701 of the mothers reported on a health questionnaire that they’d had one or more fevers while pregnant. The team followed all of the participants’ children until they reached age 9, on average, and found that 583 received an autism diagnosis.

The timing of the fever matters for autism risk, the researchers found. Compared with the 40 percent increase in the second trimester, having a fever in the first trimester carries a 34 percent increase in the risk of autism, but that result is not statistically significant. Having a fever in the third semester has no effect on autism risk.

The researchers also found a dose-response relationship for fever: The increase in risk ranges from 30 percent throughout pregnancy for one or two episodes of fever, to more than threefold for three or more episodes in the second trimester and beyond.

Still, the vast majority of women who have a fever during pregnancy do not have a child with autism and the absolute increase in risk is small. Even among mothers who had three or more fevers, only 5 out of 308 children (about 1.6 percent) have autism, compared with 376 of 65,502 children (about 0.6 percent) whose mothers reported no fevers. . . .
Hornig’s team examined whether medications that lower body temperature would reduce the risk. More than 5,600 women took acetaminophen for fever during their second trimester. The team found an association between acetaminophen use and a decrease in autism risk, but it was not statistically significant. Only 161 women took ibuprofen during the second trimester, and none of them have a child with autism. (About half of pregnant women use acetaminophen at least once, but doctors generally advise against ibuprofen use during pregnancy.) . . .
A 2013 study led by Hertz-Picciotto found that drugs that lower fever mitigate the increase in autism risk associated with fever. 
The mechanism for fevers to cause autism in a fetus might include autoimmune responses and/or inflammation (which aren't mutually exclusive - inflammation is one type of autoimmune response).

Autism has a strong genetic component, although it appears that a lot of the risk comes from de novo mutations that were not present in either of the parents when they were born. It also appears that women have protective genes that are absent in men that mitigate autism symptoms or prevent them from arising.

Baseline autism spectrum disorder risk is on the order of 1% of all births (comprising a large share of symptoms previously classified as cognitive developmental disorders not otherwise classified), so it is closer to 1.4% percent for pregnant women who have fevers in the second trimester (a scenario consistent with the notion that the anatomical systems that develop in this time period during gestation are the ones that are associated with autism).

But, if the 40% figure is to be applied on a case by case basis, rather than across the board (which isn't clear from the media report that I link) the concern may be much more of a concern in pregnancies where this is a high risk of autism (e.g. in cases where the father is middle aged or older, where there is a family history of sub-clinical autism-like personality traits, and where the fetus is male), than in pregnancies where no autism risk factors are present. 

On the other hand, if this is an across the board increase in risk, that does not interact with other risk factors, then second trimester fevers may be the main source of risk in otherwise low autism risk pregnancies, while it is a minor consideration relative to other risk factors in high autism risk pregnancies.

This is one of only a handful of well documented environmental causes of autism, which has a strong genetic component. Another is exposure of the parents to agricultural chemicals (before conception in the case of fathers and during gestation in the case of mothers).

20 June 2017

Moderate Brain Damage Common But Not Universal In Serial Killers

Most serial killers have brain damage, but several prominent ones (e.g. Ted Bundy, Jeffrey Dahmer, and John Grady) did not. Those that do have brain damage do not have the most severe cases of brain damage, probably because that would critically impair their ability to plan and execute complex behavior.

We know that traumatic brain injury (TBI) is extremely common in the most difficult inmates. My guess is that those serial killers who do not show signs of brain damage are individuals who were born psychopaths, but who have escaped brain damage from trauma.

17 May 2017

Child Psychopathy Can Be Managed With Cutting Edge Treatment

It has been known for a while now that a significant share of individuals who as adults would be classified as pyschopaths have had that traits since at least age 3-4 and probably congenitally (the etiology of psychopathy is unknown but is believed to involve the limbic system in general and the amygdala, in particular).

A story in The Atlantic magazine discusses two programs that have been successful in helping kids with this condition manage it. It doesn't cure the condition, although there is some hope that childhood treatment may alter its progression. But, it does use psychological hacks flowing from an understanding of how psychopathy changes a person behaviorally to tame a child's destructive and dangerous tendencies, and to channel the child into more pro-social behavior.

The article is rich, credible, consistent with other writing on the topic and worth time to read, even though it is lengthy.

Until now, no really proven treatment programs existed. The condition is present in about 1% of kids, comparable in frequency to schizophrenia, bipolar personality and autism spectrum conditions. But, because its connotations are (rightfully) so horrible, there hasn't been a social movement to help those who suffer from it in the same way that there has for those other conditions. For the most part, the existing default approach has been to address this conditions through the ordinary criminal justice system, both for juveniles and adults, by imposing long periods of incarceration or capital punishment.

16 April 2017

Quote Of The Day

Important differences might not show up in brain scans. For example, we can’t see schizophrenia in a brain scan, yet schizophrenia is important. Hearing voices that aren’t there, even when they give useful tips [“They’re all against you! They all must die!”] has social significance.
From G. Cochran

13 December 2016

Autism Mutation Study Discovers The Obvious

To the surprise of no one, mutations in people with autism tend to effect the "essential genes" in a person as opposed to the not very important, you could do just fine without them, genes in the human genome. Who knew?

A paper with the same theme, but a less obvious conclusion has the following abstract and citation:
Genetic susceptibility to Intellectual disability (ID), autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and schizophrenia (SCZ) often arises from mutations in the same genes, suggesting that they share common mechanisms. We studied genes with de novo mutations in the three disorders and genes implicated by SCZ genome-wide association study (GWAS). Using biological annotations and brain gene expression, we show that mutation class explains enrichment patterns more than specific disorder. Genes with loss of function mutations and genes with missense mutations were enriched with different pathways, shared with genes intolerant to mutations. Specific gene expression patterns were found for each disorder. ID genes were preferentially expressed in fetal cortex, ASD genes also in fetal cerebellum and striatum, and genes associated with SCZ were most significantly enriched in adolescent cortex. Our study suggests that convergence across neuropsychiatric disorders stems from vulnerable pathways to genetic variations, but spatiotemporal activity of genes contributes to specific phenotypes.

14 November 2016

What Does It Take To Understand The Brain?

A provocative new think piece asks whether the experimental neuroscience program is on track by asking the question, "Could a neuroscientist understand a microprocessor?"

It finds that while our current neuroscience research agenda is broad enough, carried to its logical conclusion, to tell us a great deal, that it has serious blind spots that make it incapable of reaching key insights into the functioning of our brains with any amount of data collected in the current paradigm, no matter how much we gather. Therefore, neuroscience needs to supplement its research agenda with new approaches calculated to gain insight into qualitatively different kinds of knowledge about the brain.
There is a popular belief in neuroscience that we are primarily data limited, and that producing large, multimodal, and complex datasets will, with the help of advanced data analysis algorithms, lead to fundamental insights into the way the brain processes information. 
These datasets do not yet exist, and if they did we would have no way of evaluating whether or not the algorithmically-generated insights were sufficient or even correct. To address this, here we take a classical microprocessor as a model organism, and use our ability to perform arbitrary experiments on it to see if popular data analysis methods from neuroscience can elucidate the way it processes information. Microprocessors are among those artificial information processing systems that are both complex and that we understand at all levels, from the overall logical flow, via logical gates, to the dynamics of transistors. 
We show that the approaches reveal interesting structure in the data but do not meaningfully describe the hierarchy of information processing in the microprocessor. This suggests current analytic approaches in neuroscience may fall short of producing meaningful understanding of neural systems, regardless of the amount of data. Additionally, we argue for scientists using complex non-linear dynamical systems with known ground truth, such as the microprocessor as a validation platform for time-series and structure discovery methods. 
Eric Jons and Konrad Kording, "Could a neuroscientist understand a microprocessor?" (Pre-Print November 14, 2016). doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/055624

11 November 2016

Deep Genetic Links Exist Between Common Severe Psychiatric Conditions

A several notable genetic markers are common across schizophrenia, major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder according to a new meta-analysis.
The search for biomarkers has been one of the leading endeavours in biological psychiatry; nevertheless, in spite of hundreds of publications, hardly any marker has proved useful in clinical practice. To study how biomarker research has progressed over the years, we performed a systematic review of the literature to evaluate (a) the most studied peripheral molecular markers in major psychiatric disorders, (b) the main experimental design features of studies in which they are proposed as biomarkers and (c) whether their patterns of variation are similar across disorders. 
An automated search revealed that, out of the six molecules most commonly present as keywords in articles studying plasmatic markers of schizophrenia, major depressive disorder or bipolar disorder, five (BDNF, TNF-alpha, IL-6, C-reactive protein and cortisol) were the same across the three diagnoses. An analysis of the literature on these molecules showed that, whilst 66% of original articles compared their levels between patients and controls, only 35% were longitudinal studies, and only 10% presented an evaluation of diagnostic efficacy, a pattern that has not changed significantly over two decades. 
Interestingly, these molecules varied similarly across the three disorders, suggesting them to be nonspecific systemic consequences of psychiatric illness rather than diagnostic markers. On the basis of this, we discuss how research fragmentation between diagnoses and publication practices rewarding positive findings may be directing the biomarker literature to nonspecific targets, and what steps could be taken to increase clinical translation in the field.
Jairo V. Pinto, et al.,  "Transdiagnostic aspects of peripheral biomarkers in major psychiatric disorders: a systematic review" (pre-print November 8, 2016) doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/086124

19 October 2016

Mosaicism And Brain Disorders

Genetic epilepsy and developmental disorders in non-affected parents are sometimes de novo disorders, but are also sometimes caused by mosaicism in a parent, which means that the parent has one kind of DNA in some cells and a different kind in other cells. As a result, the risk factor genes may evade detection by normal DNA tests that only look at one, easy to access site. These variants can make their way a child's DNA in vitro after initial fertilization of the egg giving rise to a zygote.

10 May 2016

Are Autism And Schizophrenia Inverses Of Each Other?

People's brains change in a person's late teens and early twenties when they experience "synaptic pruning" that strengthens frequently used connections in the brain while trimming away superfluous ones.

In schizophrenia, this process goes overboard and the brain compensates by treating random noise as signals, which leads to hallucinations. People with autism apparently don't trim enough which impairs learning. Thus, it could be that both conditions involve defects in the same process in opposite directions.
These findings may suggest new treatments targeting GABA receptors for "normalizing" synaptic pruning in diseases such as autism and schizophrenia, where synaptic pruning is abnormal. Research has suggested that children with autism may have an over-abundance of synapses in some parts of the brain. Other research suggests that prefrontal brain areas in persons with schizophrenia have fewer neural connections than the brains of those who do not have the condition.
From here.

On the other hand, it seems odd that autism would have a very early childhood onset if it is really related to synaptic pruning.  But, there are also other developmental periods besides late puberty when synaptic pruning occurs, and perhaps an earlier time period is implicated in autism.

24 March 2016

T. Gondii Linked To Anger And Aggression

While correlation isn't causation, the data from this study is pretty suggestive and it definitely deserves further study and an attempt to replicate the result. These parasitic infections can be treated with drugs and doing so might become a standard part of an overall treatment plan for people in anger management courses who test positive for the infection.
Intermittent explosive disorder (IED) is defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, as recurrent, impulsive, problematic outbursts of verbal or physical aggression that are disproportionate to the situations that trigger them. IED is thought to affect as many as 16 million Americans, more than bipolar disorder and schizophrenia combined. . . .

Coccaro and his colleagues examined possible connections [of IED] to toxoplasmosis, an extremely common parasitic infection. Transmitted through the feces of infected cats, undercooked meat or contaminated water, toxoplasmosis is typically latent and harmless for healthy adults. However, it is known to reside in brain tissue, and has been linked to several psychiatric diseases, including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and suicidal behavior.

The research team recruited 358 adult subjects from the U.S., who were evaluated for IED, personality disorder, depression and other psychiatric disorders. Study participants were also scored on traits including anger, aggression and impulsivity. Participants fell into one of three groups. Roughly one third had IED. One third were healthy controls with no psychiatric history. The remaining third were individuals diagnosed with some psychiatric disorder, but not IED. This last group served as a control to distinguish IED from possible confounding psychiatric factors.

The research team found that IED-diagnosed group was more than twice as likely to test positive for toxoplasmosis exposure (22 percent) as measured by a blood test, compared to the healthy control group (9 percent). Around 16 percent of the psychiatric control group tested positive for toxoplasmosis, but had similar aggression and impulsivity scores to the healthy control group. IED-diagnosed subjects scored much higher on both measures than either control group.

Across all study subjects, toxoplasmosis-positive individuals scored significantly higher on scores of anger and aggression. The team noted a link between toxoplasmosis and increased impulsivity, but when adjusted for aggression scores, this link became non-significant.
From here. The source for the quoted article is Emil F. Coccaro, et al., "Toxoplasma gondii Infection." The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry (2016).

Another recent post on the effect of this infection on personality is here.  A previous post linking the infection to suicide is here.

22 February 2016

Trying To Understand Jason Dalton

The Question

I'm still trying to get my head around the Jason Brian Dalton murder spree in Kalamazoo, Michigan. He shot eight people, killing six, Saturday afternoon, following a crazed drive with an Uber passenger around 4:30 p.m.

What makes a seemingly ordinary person suddenly lose it like that?

What We Know

There Were Remarkably Few Warning Signs Until The Afternoon That The Incident Began

Apart from six tickets for speeding and two for driving without insurance/registration about fifteen to twenty years earlier, and erratic wild driving about an hour and a half before he lost it, there seems to have been absolutely no sign of what he would do.  Indeed, he ran 14 Uber runs basically without incident in the midst of his killing spree.

He had no criminal record, was married 21 years and had two kids (age 10 and 15) at age 45 without incident, was an insurance adjuster, had run 100 prior Uber runs with bad reviews but with no real serious incidents over the previous month, and came across as half-empathetic at a gun store with a friend that same weekend (how many spree shooters have in tact marriages and friends immediately prior to flipping out).  We doesn't seem to have been drunk at the time and went to an ordinary bar afterwards for a drink where he surrendered peacefully.  Authorities believe that he wasn't under the influence of drugs or alcohol.  He didn't have a history of mental illness as far as authorities can tell.  He didn't have amnesia - remembering his killings.  His moonlighting for Uber suggests that he was suffering from some financial stress, but there were no social media manifestos or recollections of anti-social behavior from neighbors.  Yes, he apparently owned a pistol (probably legally) and some long guns.  Perhaps his love of car repair could indicate a faint autistic syndrome but that's not a great fit to a long marriage, job as an insurance adjuster for many years and accounts of empathetic behavior in a gun store earlier that weekend.

Dalton played football, was on the track team and was on the wrestling team in high school where he grew up in Indiana.

Dalton was apparently a car nut, something that figured into his work life as well:
Gary Pardo Jr., whose parents live across the street from Dalton in Kalamazoo Township, described him as a family man who seemed fixated on cars and often worked on them. 
"He would go a month without mowing his lawn but was very meticulous with his cars," Pardo said, explaining that Dalton, at times, owned a Chevrolet Camaro and two Hummer SUVs. 
Progressive Insurance confirmed that he once worked for the company before leaving in 2011. Dalton was an insurance adjuster who did auto-body estimates and once taught an auto-body repair class at an area community college, said James Block, who has lived next door to him for 17 years. 
"He loved to do things outside with his kids" like taking them for rides on his lawn tractor, Block said.
His family is distancing themselves from him and his horrible acts in a statement released by attorneys whom they hired.  Law enforcement and neighbors had initially feared for their lives, but Dalton's family was not harmed, although they may have left the family home for their own safety once they were altered by the sheriff.

Neighbors did say the Dalton had been acting paranoid before the shootings. James Block, a neighbor of Dalton’s noted the suspect increasingly referencing his gun in recent days while maintaining general even handedness.

It may have been Dalton's wedding anniversary, although the reports on this point seem to be inconsistent.  He has no known connection to terrorist or extremist groups according to law enforcement sources.

Was This A Three-Phase Crime?

It kind of seems like there were two phases - one manic, racing, wild and maybe angry with an incredibly reckless 4:30 p.m. drive and then his first shooting at about 5:42 p.m. at a townhouse of a woman, Tiana Carruthers (who is expected to survive), multiple times in front of her children in an apartment complex parking lot, and then another tired, cold, and withdrawn four hours later after giving multiple Uber riders, closer to 10 p.m. when he carried out other murders, took another ride or two, and then quit and had a drink at a downtown pub eventually surrendering and confessing a couple hours after that, with a long break of dissociated "normal" during which he ran a dozen Uber rides, in between with him lashing out the second time in part because he knew the situation was hopeless after his first attempted killing that he may have believed resulted in a death in front of witnesses who could identify him eventually.

None of the shootings seem to be connected to the Uber fares.

The wild ride didn't begin at the start of the ride.  It was apparently triggered by a phone call, followed about an hour later by the townhouse shooting which might have been in reaction to that phone call somehow.
Matt Mellen told CNN he rode in Dalton's car just before the shootings started. "We got about a mile from my house, and he received a telephone call," Mellen told CNN. . . . Dalton told the caller he had a passenger in the car and would call the person back, according to Mellen. 
"Once he hung up with that phone call is when he started driving erratically," Mellen said, recounting side-swiping a car, running a stop sign and red lights. 
"I was pleading with him to stop the vehicle so he could let me out. He was surprisingly calm the whole time." 
Mellen said he was able to jump out of the car and call 911.
According to Mellen: "He said Dalton introduced himself as "Me-Me" and had a dog in the backseat."

One could imagine an affair about to be revealed or some other threatened revelation that Dalton believed would imminently destroy his life spurring the crazy driving and subsequent first shooting. If it turns out that Dalton was not a stranger to this woman, that will explain a lot.  My intuition is that once the relationship between Tiana Carruthers and Jason Brian Dalton (if any) is known, sequence of events will become a lot more clear.

A news report provides some hints, suggesting that the woman may have perhaps have been targeted out of frustration, when the true target was "Mazy". He may have been a stranger to her, but he was not a stranger to the apartment complex.  It may be relevant that Carruthers and many other residents of the complex were African-American, while Dalton was white.
The first victim of the Uber gunman threw herself in front of children when the gunman opened fire and gave police crucial information to track him down. 
Tiana Carruthers was outside her Kalamazoo, Michigan, apartment with several youngsters on a playground at around 5:00pm on Saturday when the suspect, who's been named as Jason Dalton, pulled up in his Chevrolet. 
Sensing trouble, the mother put herself between the attacker and the children, and was shot multiple times as a result, but survived and was able to give the police vital evidence that helped them catch the suspected killer. 
Joi Coleman, 12, and sister Megan were two of the children she saved. Joi was holding Carruthers' hand when Dalton started firing. 
Her actions meant the pair were able to get inside one of the houses and call 911.
Travis Gettys and Devin Fletter, who live next door to Coleman's, told WWMT they saw Dalton earlier in the day talking to children in the neighborhood just an hour before the shooting [ed. i.e. at about 4:45 p.m., shortly after the "wild ride" and phone call with Mellen].
One person said he asked where 'Mazy' was, leading some to believe he was looking for someone specific. 
When police arrived at her family's home after the shooting, they found 10 shell casings on the floor around Carruthers. 
According to MLive, Carruthers was able to give deputies a description of the man who shot her at the scene.
Another report noted suggests that Dalton was not known to Carruthers personally:
At the scene, Carruthers was able to give deputies a description of the man who shot her, describing him as an older, heavy-set white male with brown and graying hair. 
Sparrow testified that Carruthers told deputies her attacker left in a silver sport-utility vehicle. Other witnesses told deputies the vehicle was either a Chevrolet Equinox or Traverse. 
Sparrow said that deputies later learned that the vehicle, after leaving Meadows Townhomes was involved in a crash after it sideswiped another vehicle at the intersection of Gull Road and East G Avenue. 
When she was shown a police lineup after the shooting, Sparrow said Carruthers identified Dalton as the man who shot her.
So, Dalton was still in a rage when he left the complex, presumably because he hadn't found "Mazy" and had just shot someone multiple times for some reason related to his search for her, perhaps in a case of mistaken identity.  What happened next?
There was a four hour time period between the first shooting at Meadows Townhomes and the second shooting at Seelye Kia, we are told police are trying to find out what he was doing during that time period, and revealed he made phone calls to several people.
Presumably, all of those people will be interviewed as witnesses in the case.

Witnesses at the first scene think that asking for "Mazy" may have been a ploy, so Carruthers may have been the intended target after all:
"He was here to kill, like to shoot somebody.” 
That’s how Travis Gettys described the man suspected of shooting and wounding a woman at Richland Township townhomes complex . . .

At the Meadows Townhomes, tire tracks from the car taking off were still visible Sunday and spray paint marked where 10 bullet casings were found. Seven bullet holes could be seen in siding at the front of a near-by apartment.

Neighbors told 24 Hour News 8 that kids were out playing just before 6 p.m. Saturday when the shooter circled around the block before he opened fire. . . . .

Neighbors say the woman recently moved in to the Meadows and was outside with her kids when the shooter, believed to be 45-year-old Jason Dolton, asked if she was a woman with a different name. People who spoke to 24 Hour News 8 said they think it was a ploy to get her to come close to his car. 
“She was talking about her kids, talking about, ‘He asked if I was so-and-so,'” said George’s mother, Tammy. 
Witnesses say the victim’s three children were outside with her. Thankfully, they ran away and avoided being shot. 
James George and his friends, who were playing video games, also avoided injury when four of the bullets came flying through the wall of his home.
Authorities claim that the first shooting was random, despite the fragmentary evidence that seems to point to a contrary conclusion, such as the fact that Dalton came to the complex twice an hour apart and asked for a named individual before shooting, and that he seemed to have a purpose to kill from the outset.

Given the large time gap between the collision after the shooting at the apartment complex and the next time his whereabouts are known, Dalton may have spent part of that time hiding out, cooling down, or perhaps committing another murder, of "Mazy" that has not yet been discovered because there were not witnesses.  Honestly, it wouldn't change the result of the criminal prosecution in any case (except possibly to provide evidence against the argument that he could claim an insanity defense).

According to a neighbor:
Block said Dalton was home ‘between the shootings’ Saturday because his niece saw Dalton pull out of his driveway about 7 p.m. The first shooting occurred about an hour earlier.
Seven of the people shot and all of the people killed died in two incidents, about 15 minutes apart, at 10:15 p.m. and 10:30 p.m., long after Dalton had been taking people on rides for many hours following the initial non-fatal shooting at an apartment complex which took place an hour and a half after his insanely reckless Uber fare drive that his passenger warned others about to no avail.

Perhaps the call that set off the first wild ride had him deranged and unbalanced, leading him to make the first killing (which might have been less random and more impulse driven), and then he sunk into guilt after hours of experiencing no consequences for the prior shooting and wild ride - causing him to become a classic "amok" killer knowing that his ultimate capture and punishment for the first shooting was just a matter of time (a scenario that wouldn't require any drugs or neurological condition at any stage).

Maybe the later two shooting incidents were not strictly random, but settled up otherwise minor scores that had exploded out of proportion for him because he was sure he had no future in the outside world after the first shooting which he believed had been deadly.  Maybe as a automobile nerd, he killed the two Smiths because they were at a business that he believed had done him wrong in the past. Dalton seemed to have "hunted" them (as a girlfriend who was undetected watched).  Maybe he lashed out at Cracker Barrel customers because he really, really hated that restaurant and wanted to include that as a final statement to the world.  Maybe he went to the bar after that because he ran out of grudges and slights to settle and was so stunned that he'd gotten away with everything he'd done without consequences that he thought he might wake up with a hangover and find out that none of it had really happened and it was all just a bad dream.

Probably the only way that this could have been prevented in the real world would have been for police, with a decent identification from the wild ride victim who promptly called 9-1-1, to have made a prompt arrest based upon reckless driving charges.  It isn't clear why police didn't make more of an effort to arrest Dalton at this point given how insane his driving had been reported to be by Mellen (who seems to have done everything possible to avert the tragedy under the circumstances).

If the multi-stage nature of the crime can shed insight into motive, then only the first stage becomes truly incomprehensible, and some shimmer of light can be shed on how even that could happen.

Was This Pre-Meditated?

Yet, there are traces of pre-meditation.  He bought the jacket in which he concealed his pistol on a trip to a gun shop with a friend not long before the spree occurred (at about 3 p.m.), which suggests that the idea was already churning in his head at this point.  Perhaps he suspects that he was about to be betrayed and armed himself as a result in order to carry out just the kind of shooting that he did at the townhouse, and then, having extra bullets and a gun on hand, was equipped to carry out random shootings in despair afterwards.

But, maybe he bought the jacket and carried the pistol for the same self-protection reason that anyone who deals with a lot of drunk people late at night might due so, and not because he planned to go on a shooting spree.  Then, when he had a completely unanticipated psychological break, those ingredients just happened to be already in place.

The jacket may have made it feasible for him to carry the pistol with him, and then, because the pistol was present when he lost it in what otherwise would have been mere grist for the local newspaper or television police reports, became much more deadly.

Theories That Can And Cannot Be Ruled Out

Psychiatric and Neurological Conditions

Psychopathy starts to manifest in preschool and almost never emerges suddenly in middle age. Schizophrenia and bipolar usually hit you in your late teens or twenties (probably because the pruning of neurons that gives most of us more efficient but less plastic brains occurs then). ADHD manifests by elementary school and doesn't lead to the kind of cold affect he had during at least the second phase of the spree, and most "impulsive" criminals "age out" by age 45, but ADHD treatment could have afforded him access to amphetamines or something similar which in a massive OD might be consistent with the facts and with his prior spat of speeding violations at a much younger age. Traumatic brain injury (TBI) or PTSD might fit that profile, but there is nothing in his recent history to suggest he'd suffered either (or that he had any history of mental illness) - although I wouldn't rule out recent TBI (e.g. bonking himself on the head against a car or car part or tool while working on cars in his garage by himself), which often goes undetected and can happen at any time in ways that would be invisible to outsiders.  It was too sudden to be any common form of dementia. It might be consistent with a recent TIA (i.e. a mini-stroke), or a brain tumor, that selectively severed some key aspect of mental functioning in middle age (I have a relative who suffered a serious farming accident leading to some sort of stroke who had a dramatic personality change afterwards, so I can sort of see that).

Possible Substance Abuse Causes

He lacked several of the symptoms of PCP usage, and isn't consistent with heroin or marijuana use either. Prolonged sleep deprivation made possible with amphetamines or meth or cocaine that he OD'd on late that afternoon might fit the timeline of crazy to relatively normal to depressed fairly well, and incidents of "losing it" in that fact pattern are not unprecedented.  Or, maybe he could be flipping out on some other drug with which the public isn't very familiar.  But, if there was a drug, something like alcohol or barbiturates or narcotics that impair motor function don't seem like a good fit given the amount of driving and accurate shooting he did during the spree with only one ride that seemed to be really wild very early on.  On the other hand, addictions to many kinds of drugs can lead to paranoia which for a person in possession of a gun can lead to over-reactions that in turn might trigger despair and further kills later on in the spree.

More Analysis

The rapid up and down points more to some kind of PTSD or psycho-active drug, than to TBI or a TIA or a brain tumor.

On the other hand, the traces of pre-meditation suggest that the idea was already churning in his head at this point, which is inconsistent with a one time effect from drug use, although paranoia or psychosis triggered by sustained drug use might fit that profile and something like TBI or a TIA or a brain tumor is also a better fit to pre-mediation.  But, if the signs of pre-mediation were not actually pre-mediated and were instead just ordinary self-defense preparations of an otherwise sane person, that simply happened to be in place when a psychotic break occurred (which was itself perhaps caused by a one time drug OD of some type), the gun and jacket to conceal it may have simply made it a much more potent (and tragic) incident.

Overall, at first glance, this is one of the most inexplicable mass murders that I can recall.

It is really hard to make sense of it if there is no brain impairment of recent origin, or drug side effect, to explain it. This is not a classic case of going amok from the start that involves an intent to commit suicide while taking as many people as possible with you without interruption in your spree.

There is no sense from the people who interacted with him on his later Uber runs that day that he was angry so much as he was tired.

Are The Theories Sufficient To Provide A Legal Defense?

Obviously, in the drug free, neurological defect free crime of passion followed by crime of suicidal despair scenario, there is no legal defense.

It is quite likely that the substance use or neurological cause of Jason Dalton's spree, if it was one of the more plausible theories suggested above, would not be a meaningful legal defense to criminal liability in the form of life in prison without possibility of parole for Jason Dalton (which would include pretty much any sentence with a minimum parole eligibility after 35-40 years).  If he ever makes it out of prison, it will likely be based upon compassionate release as he is on the verge of dying.

Even if one of these neurological or substance based causes existed, it isn't obvious that they would exonerate him from first degree murder, because they might destroy only his willingness to conform his behavior to right and wrong and not his knowledge of right and wrong - which seems to have existed at some level based upon his denial that he was the shooter to his last fare of the night whom he left unscathed for whatever reason.

Similarly, voluntary substance use is almost never a valid legal excuse, and even if drug use downgraded his offense from first degree murder to a lesser included form of homicide by depriving him of premeditation and perhaps merely classified him as reckless or as acting in the heat of passion, the number of people killed would produce a de facto life without parole sentence in any case from consecutive sentences to the lesser included multiple homicides in this case, given his age.

Since Jason Dalton surrendered peacefully to police when they came to arrest him, since he lives in Michigan which does not have the death penalty, and since he does not appear to be denying that he committed these acts (for which there are multiple witnesses and lots of other solid evidence to support his confession such as the fact that at least two of the shootings were apparently captured on video), it isn't impossible that we may someday know why he did this, unlike so many similar incidents in which the shooter either dies in connection with the spree, or maintains a strict silence afterwards to preserve his legal capacity to appeal.

A detailed confession and/or toxicology report may provide answers.

When his only possible defense is that his mind was broken or temporarily impaired, he has every incentive to cooperate in explaining himself, and his counsel has every incentive to try to find a neurological explanation (if there is one).

Should Any Of The Theories Provide A Legal Defense?

If Jason Dalton was suffering from some recent undiagnosed neurological condition that impaired his capacity to control himself, or made the once in a lifetime mistake of ODing on some sort of stimulant while he was harmed and in a car driving perhaps while sleep deprived as well, not realizing how badly that could mess him up, he looks a lot less culpable than he would otherwise (and certainly not a good candidate for some theory to try to apply the federal death penalty to his case).

But, once we've learned what he can do with no warning, should the public ever give him any opportunity to screw up again?  This is certainly not a paradigmatic example of the insanity defense, and allowing Dalton a defense might leave too much gray area to allow a more culpable mass killer to go free, or for a jury to mischaracterize the circumstances that made him do what he did.

Deterrence Remains Irrelevant

One way that this mass killing is typical is that deterrence is irrelevant to this class of crimes including even this particularly inexplicable one.

The clearance rate in this mass killing, as in almost all mass killings except those incident to gang crimes, is basically 100%.  The killer always either dies trying or is captured and doesn't deny committing the acts.  The killer generally expects to be caught or killed at some point in the process of carrying out the crime.  The threat of prosecution resulting in a certain conviction and never leaving a correctional institution except through death is credible and always carried out when the killer survives the incident.  So, the usual logic of criminal justice is inapplicable to these cases.  The only meaningful way to deal with mass shooting that don't involve gang crimes is to prevent them (or at least, to minimize the harm that they cause by swiftly interrupting an active shooter).

Implications For Gun Control

Of course, we care about the reason so we can prevent similar incidents in the future and preserve public safety.

According to a neighbor, Dalton bought a handgun a few years ago to deal with prowlers, and he did fire it a couple of times.  One report said that Dalton had 11 rifles at his home which were not used in the spree killing. Another said he had many handguns and long guns, apparently all legally. One wonders if anyone who has that many firearms isn't a risk.

Uber prohibits both drivers and passengers from carrying firearms, on pain of being banned from the service.

But, the only gun control measure that might have been effective would have been an extreme ban on handgun possession or ownership by anyone not in law enforcement or a security profession.  Almost any other imagine concealed carry regime would have allowed Jason Dalton to carry a concealed pistol.